Skip to main content

The effects of attractive vs. repulsive instructional cuing on balance performance

Abstract

Background

Torso-based vibrotactile feedback has been shown to improve postural performance during quiet and perturbed stance in healthy young and older adults and individuals with balance impairments. These systems typically include tactors distributed around the torso that are activated when body motion exceeds a predefined threshold. Users are instructed to “move away from the vibration”. However, recent studies have shown that in the absence of instructions, vibrotactile stimulation induces small (~1°) non-volitional responses in the direction of its application location. It was hypothesized that an attractive cuing strategy (i.e., “move toward the vibration”) could improve postural performance by leveraging this natural tendency.

Findings

Eight healthy older adults participated in two non-consecutive days of computerized dynamic posturography testing while wearing a vibrotactile feedback system comprised of an inertial measurement unit and four tactors that were activated in pairs when body motion exceeded 1° anteriorly or posteriorly. A crossover design was used. On each day participants performed 24 repetitions of Sensory Organization Test condition 5 (SOT5), three repetitions each of SOT 1–6, three repetitions of the Motor Control Test, and five repetitions of the Adaptation Test. Performance metrics included A/P RMS, Time-in-zone and 95 % CI Ellipse. Performance improved with both cuing strategies but participants performed better when using repulsive cues. However, the rate of improvement was greater for attractive versus repulsive cuing.

Conclusions

The results suggest that when the cutaneous signal is interpreted as an alarm, cognition overrides sensory information. Furthermore, although repulsive cues resulted in better performance, attractive cues may be as good, if not better, than repulsive cues following extended training.

Introduction

Vibrotactile feedback has been shown to improve postural performance during quiet and perturbed stance in healthy young adults [14], older adults [1, 5, 6] and individuals with balance impairments [610]. Vibrotactile displays for balance-related applications typically include an array of tactors distributed around the head [11, 12] or the torso [9, 13, 14]. For torso-based systems, an inertial measurement unit (IMU) measures torso linear acceleration(s) and/or angular velocity(ies) and tactors are activated when body motion exceeds predefined thresholds in either the anterior/posterior (A/P) and/or medial/lateral (M/L) directions. The feedback is typically associated with the specific instruction to “move away from the vibration” (i.e., repulsive cuing strategy) [2, 1517]. These displays serve as “alarm” signals to indicate body movement away from a stable posture, and require volitional postural responses [18]. However, volitional postural responses to alarm signals may be incongruent with the kinesthetic messages from the tactile receptors stimulated. Recent studies have shown that in the absence of instruction, vibrotactile stimulation induces small (~1°) non-volitional responses in the direction of its application location when vibrations are applied to the skin over the internal oblique (IO) and erector spinae (ES) muscle locations [1820]. Therefore, we hypothesize that an attractive cuing strategy (i.e., “move toward the vibration”) leveraging this “natural” tendency may further facilitate the use of vibrotactile feedback for balance related applications. Attractive cuing has previously been used during walking [21, 22], driving [23] and flying [24] applications. The purpose of this study was to characterize the effects of cuing strategy on postural performance.

Methods

Eight healthy older adults (5 F, 3 M, 65 ± 2 years) were recruited to participate in two non-consecutive days of computerized dynamic posturography testing (Equitest, Neurocom Inc). This study was reviewed and approved by the University of Michigan Internal Review Board and all subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Subjects wore a polyester t-shirt and thin athletic socks. Four tactors (C-2, EAI Inc) were mounted in a belt with Velcro and placed over the shirt on the torso over the right and left internal oblique (front) and right and left erector spinae (back) locations (Fig. 1) at the level of the L4/L5 segment [18]. The tactors were activated in pairs (IO or ES) when the torso tilt and half the tilt rate (P + 0.5D), measured by a six degree-of-freedom IMU (MTx, XSens Inc) placed at the L3 vertebrae and sampled at 80Hz, exceeded a 1° threshold in either the anterior or posterior directions. A crossover design was used wherein half of the subjects were given repulsive cues on day 1 (move away from the vibration) and the other half of the subjects were given attractive cues on day 1 (move toward the vibration). Testing days were separated by at least one day, but were no more than one week apart.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Top: Illustration of tactor placement on the torso over the areas corresponding to the left and right internal oblique (LIO, RIO) muscles and left and right erector spinae (LES, RES) muscles. Bottom: Photograph of the belt, IMU, and tactors

Prior to testing, subjects were given a minimum of 5–10 min, but not more than 15 min, of active training on how to interpret and respond to the vibrotactile feedback during balance testing. Participants were told to “initiate a postural correction (toward or away) when you feel the vibration”. Participants performed the same set of tasks on each testing day. The experimental protocol included three distinct parts: 1) 24 repetitions of Sensory Organization Test (SOT) condition 5 (sway referenced support surface, eyes closed); 2) three repetitions each of SOT 1 – 6; 3) three repetitions the Motor Control Test (MCT with large back translations, eyes closed) and five repetitions of the Adaptation Test (ADT with large toes up rotations, eyes closed). Data from parts 1 and 3 are included in the present study.

Performance metrics included root mean square (RMS) sway in the A/P direction (A/P RMS), percentage time during each trial within a 1° no feedback zone (Time-in-Zone), and a 95th percentile confidence interval ellipse fit to the sway data (95 % CI Ellipse). For the ADT and MCT tests additional performance metrics included maximum A/P tilts and the percentage of trials in which subjects returned to the no feedback zone after the perturbation within the collected time.  Cuing strategy preferences were collected using a questionnaire.

Data were processed using Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA). IMU raw data were low-pass filtered using a 2nd order Butterworth filter with a cutoff of 10 Hz [25]. The natural log of all metrics was used to ensure normal distributions. Since there were multiple outcomes per participant, a mixed effect model with a random intercept was used to determine the effects of repulsive and attractive cuing strategy, and the carry-over effects between testing days. The learning effects across the 24 SOT trials were compared by performing a regression analysis on the logarithmically transformed data and examining the interaction effects between trial and cuing strategy. The mixed effect analyses were performed using Stata 13.1 (StataCorp 2013).

At the end of the second testing day user preferences were collected with a six-question survey. Questions included two Likert-style questions (e.g., “I found repulsive cues easy to use”) and four fill in the blank questions (e.g., “I performed the balance task the best with ____ cuing”).

Results

According to all metrics, postural performance for SOT 5 was better with repulsive cuing compared to attractive cuing (Fig. 2). Sway in the A/P direction (A/P RMS) was significantly smaller (~1°) for repulsive versus attractive cues (p < 0.001); A/P RMS values were also significantly smaller (~1°) on Day 2 versus Day 1 (p < 0.001) with minimal carry over effects between the two days (p = 0.9). The 95 % CI Ellipse values were significantly smaller (~1 deg2) for repulsive versus attractive cues (p = 0.004) and significantly smaller (~1 deg2) on Day 2 versus Day 1 (p = 0.002) with minimal carry over effects (p = 0.2). Subjects spent slightly more time (~1 %) in the no feedback zone (Time-in-Zone) when using repulsive cues (p < 0.001) and slightly more time (~1 %) in the no feedback zone on Day 2 compared to Day 1 (p = 0.003) with minimal carry over effects (p = 0.6). The slope of the regression analysis fit to the logarithmically transformed A/P RMS data (not shown) was steeper (i.e., more negative) for attractive versus repulsive cues, however it was not significant (p = 0.2). The slope of the fit to the logarithmically transformed Time-in-Zone data (not shown) was steeper (i.e., more positive) for attractive versus repulsive cues (p = 0.002). The slopes of the fits to the logarithmically transformed A/P RMS and 95 % CI Ellipse data significantly decreased as a function of trial (p = 0.001) regardless of day or cuing strategy (no interaction effects).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Group averages for each trial of SOT 5 for repulsive (blue) and attractive (red) cuing (±SE) for (a) A/P RMS tilt, (b) Time-in-zone and (c) 95 % CI Ellipse fit to the A/P vs M/L tilts

Peak A/P tilts for the MCT and ADT perturbations (Fig. 3) were significantly smaller for all conditions with repulsive cuing (p = 0.001). Also, the percentage of trials in which subjects returned to the no feedback zone after the perturbation was significantly larger for repulsive cuing versus attractive cuing (Fig 3, p = 0.01).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Group averages of peak A/P tilts for (a) MCT and (b) ADT trials for repulsive (blue) and attractive (red) cuing (±SE). Percentages on graphs indicate percent of trials in which subjects returned to the no-feedback zone after perturbation within the collected time period for repulsive and attractive cuing

From the survey questionnaire, five subjects preferred repulsive cuing and two preferred attractive cuing (one had no preference) and all eight subjects said that they learned to use repulsive cuing the quickest.

Discussion

Performance improved with both cuing strategies for all conditions (SOT, MCT, ADT); however, participants performed better when using repulsive cues regardless of whether they completed repulsive cuing trials on the first or second day of testing. The regression analysis demonstrated that participants’ rates of improvement were greater for attractive versus repulsive cuing for the A/P RMS sway and Time-in-Zone metrics.

The results do not fully support our initial hypothesis based on the benefit of congruence between sensory and cognitive information. It appears that when a sensory signal is attributed an “alarm” significance, the corresponding messages are interpreted according to the cognitive assignation and not as the natural feedback. This suggests that avoidance behavior, derived from life experience and/or default sensorimotor pathways as in the withdrawal reflex, prevail over apparently conflicting sensory information. It may be hypothesized that the cognitive system resolves a possible conflict by choosing a low cost (known) solution that may not be optimal in terms of response time. The lower initial performance with attractive cuing, particularly when it is presented on Day 1 (Group 2) may be indicative of a cognitive “incongruence” engendered by the less natural/common requirement of attraction toward a perturbation. Although in the end both strategies led to a similar level of performance, the findings would tend to support the use of a repulsive cuing strategy when time to learn to use a torso-based vibrotactile feedback system is limited. However, based on the steeper slopes of the fits to the logarithmically transformed SOT 5 data (not shown in the figures), performance using attractive cues following a training period may be as good, if not better, than performance using repulsive cues. Future work should involve extended within and across session training protocols.

Abbreviations

A/P:

anterior/posterior

ADT:

adaptation test

CI:

confidence interval

ES:

erector spinae

IO:

internal oblique

IMU:

inertial measurement unit

MCT:

motor control test

M/L:

medial/lateral

RMS:

root mean square

SOT:

sensory organization test

References

  1. Davis JR, Carpenter MG, Tschanz R, Meyes S, Debrunner D, Burger J, et al. Trunk sway reductions in young and older adults using multi-modal biofeedback. Gait Posture. 2010;31(4):465–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Wall 3rd C, Weinberg MS, Schmidt PB, Krebs DE. Balance prosthesis based on micromechanical sensors using vibrotactile feedback of tilt. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2001;48(10):1153–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lee BC, Kim J, Chen S, Sienko KH. Cell phone based balance trainer. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2012;9:10. 10-0003-9-10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Vuillerme N, Pinsault N, Chenu O, Boisgontier M, Demongeot J, Payan Y. How a plantar pressure-based, tongue-placed tactile biofeedback modifies postural control mechanisms during quiet standing. Exp Brain Res. 2007;181(4):547–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Haggerty S, Jiang LT, Galecki A, Sienko KH. Effects of biofeedback on secondary-task response time and postural stability in older adults. Gait Posture. 2012;35(4):523–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Allum JH, Carpenter MG, Horslen BC, Davis JR, Honegger F, Tang KS, et al. Improving impaired balance function: real-time versus carry-over effects of prosthetic feedback. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2011;2011:1314–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Wall 3rd C, Kentala E. Control of sway using vibrotactile feedback of body tilt in patients with moderate and severe postural control deficits. J Vestib Res. 2005;15(5–6):313–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Kentala E, Vivas J, Wall 3rd C. Reduction of postural sway by use of a vibrotactile balance prosthesis prototype in subjects with vestibular deficits. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2003;112(5):404–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sienko KH, Balkwill MD, Oddsson LI, Wall C. Effects of multi-directional vibrotactile feedback on vestibular-deficient postural performance during continuous multi-directional support surface perturbations. J Vestib Res. 2008;18(5–6):273–85.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Sienko KH, Vichare VV, Balkwill MD, Wall 3rd C. Assessment of vibrotactile feedback on postural stability during pseudorandom multidirectional platform motion. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2010;57(4):944–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Goebel JA, Sinks BC, Parker Jr BE, Richardson NT, Olowin AB, Cholewiak RW. Effectiveness of head-mounted vibrotactile stimulation in subjects with bilateral vestibular loss: a phase 1 clinical trial. Otol Neurotol. 2009;30(2):210–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Basta D, Rossi-Izquierdo M, Soto-Varela A, Greters ME, Bittar RS, Steinhagen-Thiessen E, et al. Efficacy of a vibrotactile neurofeedback training in stance and gait conditions for the treatment of balance deficits: a double-blind, placebo-controlled multicenter study. Otol Neurotol. 2011;32(9):1492–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Peterka RJ, Wall C 3rd, Kentala E. Determining the effectiveness of a vibrotactile balance prosthesis. J Vestib Res. 2006;16(1-2):45–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Horak FB, Dozza M, Peterka R, Chiari L, Wall 3rd C. Vibrotactile biofeedback improves tandem gait in patients with unilateral vestibular loss. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1164:279–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Sienko KH, Balkwill MD, Wall 3rd C. Biofeedback improves postural control recovery from multi-axis discrete perturbations. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2012;9:53. 53-0003-9-53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Bechly KE, Carender WJ, Myles JD, Sienko KH. Determining the preferred modality for real-time biofeedback during balance training. Gait Posture. 2013;37(3):391–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Janssen LJ, Verhoeff LL, Horlings CG, Allum JH. Directional effects of biofeedback on trunk sway during gait tasks in healthy young subjects. Gait Posture. 2009;29(4):575–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lee BC, Martin BJ, Sienko KH. Directional postural responses induced by vibrotactile stimulations applied to the torso. Exp Brain Res. 2012;222(4):471–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Lee BC, Martin BJ, Sienko KH. The effects of actuator selection on non-volitional postural responses to torso-based vibrotactile stimulation. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2013;10(1):21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Lee BC, Martin BJ, Ho A, Sienko KH. Postural reorganization induced by torso cutaneous covibration. J Neurosci. 2013;33(18):7870–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ross DA, Blasch BB. Wearable interfaces for orientation and wayfinding. Assets '00 Proceedings of the fourth international ACM conference on assistive technologies; Arlington, VA, USA, November 13-15; 2000.

  22. Bosman S, Groenendaal B, Findlater J, Visser T, de Graaf MJ, Markopoulos P. GentleGuide. An Exploration of Haptic Output for Indoors Pedestrian Guidance. Human-Computer Interaction with Mobile Devices and Services, 5th International Symposium - Mobile HCI; Udine, Italy, September 8-11, 2003.

  23. van Erp JBF, van Veen HAHC. Vibro-Tactile Information Presentation in Automobiles. EuroHaptics 2001; Birmingham, UK, July 1-4, 2001.

  24. van Veen HAHC, van Erp JBF. Tactile Information Presentation in the Cockpit. Proceedings of the First International Workshop on Haptic Human-Computer Interaction; Glasgow, UK, August 31-September 1, 2001.

  25. Winter DA. A.B.C. (Anatomy, Biomechanics, and Control) of balance during standing and walking. Waterloo: University of Waterloo; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation (GARDE 1159635 and CAREER RAPD-0846471) and the University of Michigan Pepper Center (AG024824). The authors acknowledge Andrzej T. Galecki for assistance with the statistical analysis. A preliminary version of these data were presented at the annual meeting of the International Society for Gait and Posture Research (ISPGR) from June 28th – July 2nd, 2015 in Seville Spain.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kathleen H. Sienko.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

CK participated in experimental design, recruited subjects, collected data, assisted with analysis and interpretation, and drafted the manuscript. JL created custom data collection hardware and software, and participated in data collection and analysis. WC participated in experimental design, recruited subjects, assisted with data interpretation and edited the manuscript. MK ran statistical analyses and assisted with data interpretation and edited the manuscript. BM participated in experimental design and data interpretation, and participated in writing the manuscript. KS oversaw the project, participated in experimental design, data analysis and interpretation, and writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kinnaird, C., Lee, J., Carender, W.J. et al. The effects of attractive vs. repulsive instructional cuing on balance performance. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 13, 29 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-016-0131-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-016-0131-z

Keywords