Skip to main content

Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning

Abstract

The study of gait variability, the stride-to-stride fluctuations in walking, offers a complementary way of quantifying locomotion and its changes with aging and disease as well as a means of monitoring the effects of therapeutic interventions and rehabilitation. Previous work has suggested that measures of gait variability may be more closely related to falls, a serious consequence of many gait disorders, than are measures based on the mean values of other walking parameters. The Current JNER series presents nine reports on the results of recent investigations into gait variability. One novel method for collecting unconstrained, ambulatory data is reviewed, and a primer on analysis methods is presented along with a heuristic approach to summarizing variability measures. In addition, the first studies of gait variability in animal models of neurodegenerative disease are described, as is a mathematical model of human walking that characterizes certain complex (multifractal) features of the motor control's pattern generator. Another investigation demonstrates that, whereas both healthy older controls and patients with a higher-level gait disorder walk more slowly in reduced lighting, only the latter's stride variability increases. Studies of the effects of dual tasks suggest that the regulation of the stride-to-stride fluctuations in stride width and stride time may be influenced by attention loading and may require cognitive input. Finally, a report of gait variability in over 500 subjects, probably the largest study of this kind, suggests how step width variability may relate to fall risk. Together, these studies provide new insights into the factors that regulate the stride-to-stride fluctuations in walking and pave the way for expanded research into the control of gait and the practical application of measures of gait variability in the clinical setting.

Introduction

Like most physiologic signals, measures of gait are not constants but rather fluctuate with time and change from one stride to the next, even when environmental and external conditions are fixed (Figure 1). In healthy adults, these stride-to-stride fluctuations are relatively small and the coefficient of variation of many gait parameters (e.g., gait speed, stride time) is on the order of just a few percent [1–3], testimony to the accuracy and reliability of the fine-tuned systems that regulate gait. Recently, the apparently "noisy" variations in stride length, stride time and gait speed have also been shown to display a hidden and unexpected fractal-like property [4–9]. These properties of gait exhibit long-range (power-law) correlations and a "memory" effect, such that fluctuations at any given moment are statistically related to those that occur over many different time scales. When the systems regulating gait are disturbed (e.g., as a result of certain diseases), movement control may be impaired leading to increased stride-to-stride fluctuations and/or alterations in their multiscale dynamics.

Figure 1
figure 1

Example of the stride-to-stride fluctuations in the stride time as measured in two older adults: an older adult non-faller and an idiopathic faller. In both subjects, the stride time changes from one stride to the next. Although the mean values of the stride time are essentially identical in both subjects, the magnitude of the stride-to-stride fluctuations is much larger in the faller. SD: standard deviation; CV: coefficient of variation.

The current series of the Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation (JNER) is dedicated to gait variability. As guest editor of a collection of nine papers on this topic, I have had the opportunity to preview the wealth of information on stride-to-stride fluctuations in gait and the manifold ways in which gait variability may be analyzed. The articles in this collection cover a wide spectrum of themes ranging from methods for evaluating gait variability, animal and mathematical models investigating the factors that influence the variability of gait, and evaluations of the clinical utility of such measures. Altogether, these reports underscore the complex and fascinating nature of gait variability.

To set the stage, it is helpful to briefly highlight previous work in this area. Earlier studies have demonstrated that:

• Gait variability is a quantifiable feature of walking that is altered (both in terms of magnitude and dynamics) in clinically relevant syndromes, such as falling, frailty, and neuro-degenerative disease (e.g., Parkinson's and Alzheimer's disease [10–19].

• The magnitude of the stride-to-stride fluctuations in stride length and step timing are unaltered in healthy older adults, whereas the dynamics of gait change with healthy aging (e.g., alterations in the fractal pattern) [1, 20, 21].

• Physiologic factors that affect gait dynamics include neural control, muscle function and postural control; however, more subtle alterations in underlying physiology including cardiovascular changes and mental health may also influence the variability of gait (Figure 2) [10–12, 16, 19, 22–24].

Figure 2
figure 2

Simplified block diagram of the locomotor system. Also shown are a sample of the alterations that occur in aging and disease which affect gait stability, at least as reflected in stride time variability, and fall risk. CBF: cerebral blood flow. Modified from Hausdorff et al, J Appl Physiol 2001.

• Improvements in muscle function and therapeutic interventions are associated with enhanced gait stability, but not always with more conventional measures of average gait velocity or cadence [12, 16, 25].

• Gait instability measures apparently predict falls in idiopathic elderly fallers and other populations who share an increased fall risk [2, 16, 17, 19, 26–30].

Thus, gait variability may serve as a sensitive and clinically relevant parameter in the evaluation of mobility, fall risk and the response to therapeutic interventions.

Gait variability: a marker of fall risk

Studies of gait variability have been motivated by a number of factors. One intriguing aspect of gait variability is its relationship to fall risk. In one of the first quantitative studies of gait variability, Guimaraes and Isaacs [31] suggested that elderly fallers walked with increased gait variability, both in terms of step length and step time, compared to non-falling older adults. Indeed, one of the "holy grails" of geriatric and rehabilitation research is the identification of markers that can be used to prospectively identify older adults at greatest risk of falling. A number of studies have demonstrated that measures of gait variability may be help achieve this end [26, 27, 29]. Indeed, survival analyses have also shown that subjects are significantly more likely to fall sooner if gait instability measures are relatively increased at baseline, further underscoring the potential utility of such measures.

The nature of the relationships among the average gait speed, the average stride length, and the variability of these measures are critical to the study of fall risk. Although a reduced gait speed has often been viewed as a sign of fall risk, Maki showed that, at least among certain older adults, average gait speed and related measures are related to fear of falling, but not to the risk of falling per se, while measures of variability predict future falls [27]. A number of other investigations demonstrated that the degree of variability may be more closely related to fall risk than average gait speed, average stride length, and average stride time [2, 26–29]. These results suggest that measures of gait variability may sometimes be more sensitive than other measures of gait, and that these measures may provide a clinical index of gait instability and fall risk. If one views gait variability as a reflection of the inconsistency in the central neuromuscular control system's ability to regulate gait and maintain a steady walking pattern, then it makes sense that measures of gait variability would be associated with instability and fall risk. A more variable gait in which the center of pressure moves over and beyond the base-of-support in a relatively uncontrolled, unstable fashion may predispose to unsteadiness and falls.

Similarly, it is important to stress that just as the assessment of the magnitude of gait variability may provide important, independent information above and beyond average values, so, too, may the investigation of the dynamics of gait variability offer additional insights. A number of studies have demonstrated this concept. Here, we briefly describe one example in which going beyond the first (the mean) and second (the standard deviation) moments proves relevant to the understanding of a disorder.

The cause of impaired gait among many older adults defies identification, even after thorough examination. This has been termed a "higher-level gait disorder" (HLGD) or "cautious gait" [32, 33]. A study of the gait dynamics of these patients found that they had significantly larger (p < .0001) gait variability (the 2nd moment) compared to controls [19] and that about 50% of them reported falling. A fractal scaling index of gait was useful in discriminating fallers from non-fallers in this patient group, while all other measures (of muscle function, balance, and gait, including gait speed and stride time variability) did not [19]. These findings illustrate how going beyond conventional statistical summaries may improve discriminatory power and provide a more complete characterization of gait changes.

In the present JNER series, Brach and colleagues study the 2nd moment to quantify the magnitude of stride-to-stride fluctuations and examine the relationship between gait variability and fall history in a population-based sample of more than 500 older adults. In what is probably the largest quantitative study on this question to date, too much or too little step width variability was associated with a fall history in a relatively healthy cohort of older adults who do not walk slowly (i.e., gait speed ≥1.0 m/sec). These findings raise a number of interesting questions about the relationship between variability and fall risk, and encourage the study of specific aspects of variability and their inter-relationships (e.g., step length vs. step width).

Gait variability and heart rate variability

The strides in knowledge gleaned from studies of other physiologic systems, particularly those on heart rate variability, have also provided valuable incentive to similarly investigate gait variability [4, 34–43] (see also http://www.physionet.org). The healthy heartbeat was originally thought to be quite regular and periodic, essentially the product of a single, metronomic pacemaker. Thus, for a long time, mean heart rate was regarded as the primary outcome, and fluctuations about the mean were largely ignored. It emerged from later studies, however, that the heart rate normally fluctuates, over many time scales, in a complex manner from one beat to the next [37, 44]. In fact, the cardiovascular system shows erratic beat-to-beat fluctuations resembling those found in dynamical systems that are being driven away from a single equilibrium state, even under entirely healthy, resting conditions. A large body of investigations have demonstrated that there is important information hidden in the dynamics of the heart rate that can be detected using methods that examine the variability, scaling and multi-scaling properties of the heartbeat [4, 39, 45]. Moreover, numerous investigations have demonstrated the clinical utility of heart rate variability measures with important diagnostic and prognostic utility including the prediction of life threatening arrhythmias and mortality [46–53].

While there are obvious fundamental differences between the regulation of heart rate and the regulation of gait, the success of research into the former has spurred dynamical investigations of the latter. In the past, the fluctuations in gait were largely ignored or erroneously viewed simply as "noise". Many of the tools for quantifying heart rate variability were applied to study the stride-to-stride fluctuations in gait [5, 6, 8, 13, 19, 35, 36, 54–56]. Of course, while both signals do share many of the same characteristics, there are several important differences: for example, increased stride time variability (i.e., the magnitude of the fluctuations) is usually a sign of pathology, while increased heart rate variability is a healthy sign. On the other hand, many of the dynamic properties of both signals are similar: heart rate and gait timing exhibit complex fluctuations reminiscent of fractals, and this property is typically altered with aging and certain diseases [4, 9, 19, 20, 47, 48, 54, 57–59]. Challenging reports to the contrary [60], in the current series, the findings of West and Latka suggest that gait fluctuations, like the healthy heart rate, are also multi-fractal.

The parallel between gait fluctuations and heart rate variability should be considered with some caution. It would be remiss to investigate heart rate variability and not examine the average heart rate. Similarly, it would be deficient to study gait variability and disregard mean values of stride time, stride length and gait speed. These measures offer an excellent, initial description of a person's mobility and gait [61]. The lesson from the study of heart rate is that additional information can be uncovered by examining the fluctuations around the means, both in terms of the magnitude and the dynamics. The experience with heart rate also poses a challenge: pharmacologic and intervention studies have clearly identified key components that underlie the fluctuations in heart rate (e.g., the interplay between the parasympathetic and sympathetic systems). Equally fundamental studies are needed to more completely understand the physiology and patho-physiology that underlie gait variability and its dynamics.

Methods: data acquisition and signal processing

Data acquisition and signal processing are two key areas that enable the study of gait variability. Traditional camera-based, motion analysis limits the study to a few strides and is not optimal for measuring the stride-to-stride fluctuations. A number of methods have been used to study gait under ambulatory conditions, including accelerometers, gyroscopes, foot switches, body-worn sensors and wearable computers, gait mats, and force-plate mounted treadmills or optical measurement of treadmill walking [27–29, 54, 62–70]. In the present series of JNER papers, Terrier and Schutz review the use of global position satellite monitoring for measuring gait. Although its time may not yet have come for routine use, this method has some important benefits, such as allowing for the determination of both the spatial and temporal measures of gait on a stride-to-stride basis.

Once the signal is acquired, questions about signal processing inevitably follow. Chau and colleagues describe challenges that arise when analyzing gait variability and present an interesting strategy for dealing with them. Their excellent review introduces the reader to different sources of variability and provides a heuristic method for summarizing various types of variability measures.

Modeling of gait variability

A number of approaches may be applied to make sense of the various measures of gait variability. In this JNER series, Amende and colleagues report on the dynamics of gait in mouse models of Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. In this first-ever study of the stride-to-stride fluctuations of gait in animal models of neurodegenerative processes, they demonstrate that gait changes parallel those seen in clinical studies of humans (check out the gait of these animals in on-line video). This finding supports the validity of these models and sets the stage for a novel means of studying gait dynamics. While there are of course critical differences between two and four legged locomotion, these animal models enable manipulation and invasive intervention that are not feasible in human studies, thus offering a way to identify the mechanisms that underlie changes in the stride-to-stride regulation of gait.

West and Latka take a different, complementary approach toward understanding the fluctuations in gait. Using mathematical methods, they build upon earlier nonlinear dynamics models of the fluctuations in the stride time [56, 71, 72] and demonstrate that these fluctuations in healthy subjects can be described using a fractional Langevin equation. It remains to be seen whether this model can be applied to data collected in animal models and how disease and aging alter model parameters.

Gait variability, cognitive function, meaning and more

Another approach taken to gain insight into the factors that influence gait variability is to manipulate the locomotor system or specific components of the system by means of clinical studies. A priori, one might argue that stride-to-stride variability is regulated by automated processes and requires minimal cognitive resources. This argument is consistent with the report of Maki [27], demonstrating that variability was related to fall risk, but not to fear of falling. Indeed, studies of dual tasking found that gait speed slowed when healthy subjects, young and old, performed a secondary dual task during walking, while the variability of stride and swing timing was unchanged, even when subjects simultaneously walked and subtracted 7's serially, a challenging cognitive task [73, 74]. In contrast, dual tasking not only reduced gait speed, it also increased variability among patients with impaired automaticity (e.g., Parkinson's disease patients) [17, 73–75]. These findings are in line with the view that the regulation of variability is normally automated and requires minimal cognitive input. However, when automaticity is impaired (e.g., in the presence of pathology, cognitive tasks affect gait variability. One recent investigation disputed the concept of automatic regulation and suggested that stride time variability is related to specific cognitive processes, namely executive function [76]. In the present series, papers by Beauchet and colleagues and Grabiner and Troy describe the effects of a secondary, dual task on the gait variability in healthy young adults. One study suggests that there is no effect on stride length variability, while there is a small increase in stride time variability due to changes in mean gait speed. The second paper suggests that stride width variability becomes reduced during dual tasking. These interesting findings raise the question: "why?" and call for a more all-embracing understanding of the mechanisms that control gait variability and a "smooth" gait.

When dealing with this question, the complex relationships between gait speed and measures of variability of gait should be considered. When all other variables are kept constant, studies in young adults have demonstrated a U-shaped relationship between stride length (speed and/or cadence) and measures of gait variability. Minimal variability occurred near the usual walking speed and cadence [77–79], where energy costs of walking are also minimal and head stability is maximal [80, 81]. Thus, when investigating gait and the factors that influence variability, it is important to take into account the possibility that any observed group differences or responses to intervention are simply a result of changes in gait speed. In many cases, however, it is possible to demonstrate that variability parameters are regulated independently of mean values (e.g., of stride length and stride time) [78]. For example, in the present series, Kessler and colleagues show that healthy controls and patients with a HLGD reduce their stride length and walk more slowly when they are asked to walk in conditions of minimal lighting. While variability measures increased among the patients, control subjects evidenced no change, even though they did walk more slowly in near darkness.

A potential way of separating values of variability from those of mean stride length and speed is described by Frenkel-Toledo and colleagues in the present series. They show that swing time variability is larger in patients with Parkinson's disease compared to healthy controls and that swing time variability is insensitive to changes in gait speed in both groups. Perhaps this measure can be used as a speed-independent measure of variability to help to unravel the mechanisms that influence the stride-to-stride fluctuations of gait and to identify measures with clinical utility that are not influenced by gait speed. Interestingly, a recent report observed a dissociation between left and right swing time variability in patients with Parkinson's disease who have a severe gait disturbance, i.e., freezing of gait [82], further demonstrating the potential utility of measures of swing time variability.

Outstanding issues

The investigations reported in this special series on gait variability advance our understanding of an intriguing aspect of gait: the ability of the healthy neural control system to fine tune the stride-to-stride fluctuations of walking to a remarkable degree. At the same time, they delineate a number of important questions that remain to be resolved by future studies. For example, several reports highlight the differences between measures of the mean, the variance and the dynamics. A theoretical framework is needed to understand these differences. One possible explanation for the difference between the results of the study by Brach and colleagues and those of previous studies relates to the question: how much is enough? In order to obtain reliable and meaningful measures of variability, how many strides need to be studied? Owings and Grabiner [64] suggest that hundreds of strides are required to accurately estimate step kinematic variability for treadmill walking. The number needed for walking on level ground is undetermined. If variability measures are to be used in the clinic, more research is required to determine the trade-off between accuracy, reliability, validity and clinical utility.

The question of how many strides to measure highlights the need for the development of standards and reference values. Standards were set to define minimum data acquisition requirements (e.g., sampling rate) to promote research and the clinical implementation of heart rate variability measures [83]. While there may be some debate about the exact values, the defining of standards greatly enhances the quality of the data and the ability to interpret and compare studies that use a given tool. Similarly, well-established reference values and norms are needed in different age groups and populations in order to promote interpretation and clinical references. Heart rate databases in different populations, complete with annotations and medical information, are widely available (e.g., see http://www.physionet.org), significantly advancing the sharing of methods and interpretation. Similar open-access database efforts would greatly help the study of gait variability and the development of clinical measures, but this must await the establishment of minimum standards for data collection and validation of the means of comparing data from different measurement systems.

The studies by West and Latka and Brach and colleagues also return us to hypotheses originally put forth by Gabell and Nayak over two decades ago [1]. They speculated that stride time variability reflects gait timing mechanisms and the pattern generator of gait, while variability of support time and step width more closely reflect balance control. Future studies are needed to unravel the various aspects of gait variability and their nonlinear interactions (in this respect, the potential of the animal models comes to fore), to identify the mechanisms that are responsible for each of the complementary measures of the stride-to-stride fluctuations in gait, and to work out the relationship between balance control and gait variability. The basal ganglia and dopamine-sensitive networks apparently participate in the regulation of gait variability while visual feedback apparently does not play a critical role in healthy adults. We lack, however, a good understanding of the neural center(s) that generates and regulates gait timing and are left to speculate why the maintenance of gait variability may be influenced by cognitive challenges, at least certain types under specific conditions. It has become clear that more sinus rhythm heart rate variability is (generally) "good", while more stride time variability is (generally) "bad" The final words on the value and interpretation of the variability of multiple other aspects of gait (e.g. step width variability), their inter-dependence and the relationship to the variability of other motor control tasks await the results of future studies [63, 65, 68, 84–88].

Conflict of interest Statement

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

References

  1. Gabell A, Nayak US: The effect of age on variability in gait. J Gerontol 1984, 39: 662-666.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Hausdorff JM, Edelberg HK, Mitchell SL, Goldberger AL, Wei JY: Increased gait unsteadiness in community-dwelling elderly fallers. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1997, 78: 278-283. 10.1016/S0003-9993(97)90034-4

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Terrier P, Schutz Y: Variability of gait patterns during unconstrained walking assessed by satellite positioning (GPS). Eur J Appl Physiol 2003, 90: 554-561. 10.1007/s00421-003-0906-3

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Goldberger AL, Amaral LAN, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov PC, Peng CK, Stanley HE: Fractal dynamics in physiology: Alterations with disease and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2002, 99: 2466-2472. 10.1073/pnas.012579499

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Griffin L, West DJ, West BJ: Random stride intervals with memory. Journal of Biological Physics 2000, 26: 185-202. 10.1023/A:1010322406831

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. West BJ, Griffin L: Allometric control, inverse power laws and human gait. Chaos Solitons & Fractals 1999, 10: 1519-1527. 10.1016/S0960-0779(98)00149-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Hausdorff JM, Purdon PL, Peng CK, Ladin Z, Wei JY, Goldberger AL: Fractal dynamics of human gait: stability of long-range correlations in stride interval fluctuations. J Appl Physiol 1996, 80: 1448-1457.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Hausdorff JM, Peng CK, Ladin Z, Wei JY, Goldberger AL: Is walking a random walk? Evidence for long-range correlations in stride interval of human gait. J Appl Physiol 1995, 78: 349-358.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Terrier P, Turner V, Schutz Y: GPS analysis of human locomotion: Further evidence for long-range correlations in stride-to-stride fluctuations of gait parameters. Hum Mov Sci 2005, 24: 97-115. 10.1016/j.humov.2005.03.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Blin O, Ferrandez AM, Serratrice G: Quantitative analysis of gait in Parkinson patients: increased variability of stride length. J Neurol Sci 1990, 98: 91-97. 10.1016/0022-510X(90)90184-O

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Hausdorff JM, Cudkowicz ME, Firtion R, Wei JY, Goldberger AL: Gait variability and basal ganglia disorders: stride-to-stride variations of gait cycle timing in Parkinson's disease and Huntington's disease. Mov Disord 1998, 13: 428-437. 10.1002/mds.870130310

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hausdorff JM, Nelson ME, Kaliton D, Layne JE, Bernstein MJ, Nuernberger A, Singh MA: Etiology and modification of gait instability in older adults: a randomized controlled trial of exercise. J Appl Physiol 2001, 90: 2117-2129.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Hausdorff JM, Lertratanakul A, Cudkowicz ME, Peterson AL, Kaliton D, Goldberger AL: Dynamic markers of altered gait rhythm in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J Appl Physiol 2000, 88: 2045-2053.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Hausdorff JM, Zemany L, Peng C, Goldberger AL: Maturation of gait dynamics: stride-to-stride variability and its temporal organization in children. J Appl Physiol 1999, 86: 1040-1047.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Hausdorff JM, Schaafsma JD, Balash Y, Bartels AL, Gurevich T, Giladi N: Impaired regulation of stride variability in Parkinson's disease subjects with freezing of gait. Exp Brain Res 2003, 149: 187-194.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Schaafsma JD, Giladi N, Balash Y, Bartels AL, Gurevich T, Hausdorff JM: Gait dynamics in Parkinson's disease: relationship to Parkinsonian features, falls and response to levodopa. J Neurol Sci 2003, 212: 47-53. 10.1016/S0022-510X(03)00104-7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sheridan PL, Solomont J, Kowall N, Hausdorff JM: Influence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer's disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 2003, 51: 1633-1637. 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51516.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Stolze H, Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP, Drucke H, Johnk K, Illert M, Deuschl G: Comparative analysis of the gait disorder of normal pressure hydrocephalus and Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001, 70: 289-297. 10.1136/jnnp.70.3.289

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Herman T, Giladi N, Gurevich T, Hausdorff JM: Gait instability and fractal dynamics of older adults with a "cautious" gait: why do certain older adults walk fearfully? Gait Posture 2005, 21: 178-185. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2004.01.014

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hausdorff JM, Mitchell SL, Firtion R, Peng CK, Cudkowicz ME, Wei JY, Goldberger AL: Altered fractal dynamics of gait: reduced stride-interval correlations with aging and Huntington's disease. J Appl Physiol 1997, 82: 262-269.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Owings TM, Grabiner MD: Step width variability, but not step length variability or step time variability, discriminates gait of healthy young and older adults during treadmill locomotion. J Biomech 2004, 37: 935-938. 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2003.11.012

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Hausdorff JM, Peng CK, Goldberger AL, Stoll AL: Gait unsteadiness and fall risk in two affective disorders: a preliminary study. BMC Psychiatry 2004, 4: 39. 10.1186/1471-244X-4-39

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Hausdorff JM, Herman T, Baltadjieva R, Gurevich T, Giladi N: Balance and gait in older adults with systemic hypertension. Am J Cardiol 2003, 91: 643-645. 10.1016/S0002-9149(02)03332-5

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Hausdorff JM, Forman DE, Ladin Z, Goldberger AL, Rigney DR, Wei JY: Increased walking variability in elderly persons with congestive heart failure. J Am Geriatr Soc 1994, 42: 1056-1061.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Frenkel-Toledo S, Giladi N, Peretz C, Herman T, Gruendlinger L, Hausdorff JM: Treadmill walking as an external pacemaker to improve gait rhythm and stability in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Hausdorff JM, Rios D, Edelberg HK: Gait variability and fall risk in community-livingolder adults: a 1-year prospective study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001, 82: 1050-1056. 10.1053/apmr.2001.24893

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Maki BE: Gait changes in older adults: predictors of falls or indicators of fear. J Am Geriatr Soc 1997, 45: 313-320.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Menz HB, Lord SR, Fitzpatrick RC: Acceleration patterns of the head and pelvis when walking are associated with risk of falling in community-dwelling older people. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2003, 58: M446-M452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Nakamura T, Meguro K, Sasaki H: Relationship between falls and stride length variability in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Gerontology 1996, 42: 108-113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Visser H: Gait and balance in senile dementia of Alzheimer's type. Age Ageing 1983, 12: 296-301. 10.1093/ageing/12.4.296

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Guimaraes RM, Isaacs B: Characteristics of the gait in old people who fall. Int Rehabil Med 1980, 2: 177-180.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Giladi N, Herman T, Reider-Groswasser II, Gurevich T, Hausdorff JM: Clinical characteristics of elderly patients with a cautious gait of unknown origin. J Neurol 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Nutt JG, Marsden CD, Thompson PD: Human walking and higher-level gait disorders, particularly in the elderly. Neurology 1993, 43: 268-279.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Glass L: Synchronization and rhythmic processes in physiology. Nature 2001, 410: 277-284. 10.1038/35065745

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Chau T: A review of analytical techniques for gait data. Part 2: neural network and wavelet methods. Gait & Posture 2001, 13: 102-120. 10.1016/S0966-6362(00)00095-3

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Chau T: A review of analytical techniques for gait data. Part 1: fuzzy, statistical and fractal methods. Gait & Posture 2001, 13: 49-66. 10.1016/S0966-6362(00)00094-1

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Goldberger AL: Is the normal heartbeat chaotic or homeostatic? News Physiol Sci 1991, 6: 87-91.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Goldberger AL: Non-linear dynamics for clinicians: chaos theory, fractals, and complexity at the bedside. Lancet 1996, 347: 1312-1314. 10.1016/S0140-6736(96)90948-4

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Havlin S, Buldyrev SV, Goldberger AL, Mantegna RN, Ossadnik SM, Peng CK, Simons M, Stanley HE: Fractals in biology and medicine. Chaos Solitons Fractals 1995, 6: 171-201. 10.1016/0960-0779(95)80025-C

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Stanley HE, Buldyrev SV, Goldberger AL, Goldberger ZD, Havlin S, Mantegna RN, Ossadnik SM, Peng CK, Simons M: Statistical mechanics in biology: how ubiquitous are long-range correlations? Physica A 1994, 205: 214-253. 10.1016/0378-4371(94)90502-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. West BJ, Latka M, Glaubic-Latka M, Latka D: Multifractality of cerebral blood flow. Physica A-Statistical Mechanics and Its Applications 2003, 318: 453-460. 10.1016/S0378-4371(02)01377-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Latka M, Glaubic-Latka M, Latka D, West BJ: Fractal rigidity in migraine. Chaos Solitons & Fractals 2004, 20: 165-170. 10.1016/S0960-0779(03)00440-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Peng CK, Mietus JE, Liu YH, Lee C, Hausdorff JM, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL, Lipsitz LA: Quantifying fractal dynamics of human respiration: Age and gender effects. Annals of Biomedical Engineering 2002, 30: 683-692. 10.1114/1.1481053

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Glass L: Complex cardiac rhythms. Nature 1987, 330: 695-696. 10.1038/330695a0

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Ivanov PC, Amaral LA, Goldberger AL, Havlin S, Rosenblum MG, Struzik ZR, Stanley HE: Multifractality in human heartbeat dynamics. Nature 1999, 399: 461-465. 10.1038/20924

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Goldberger AL, West BJ: Applications of nonlinear dynamics to clinical cardiology. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1987, 504: 195-213. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1987.tb48733.x

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Ho KK, Moody GB, Peng CK, Mietus JE, Larson MG, Levy D, Goldberger AL: Predicting survival in heart failure case and control subjects by use of fully automated methods for deriving nonlinear and conventional indices of heart rate dynamics. Circulation 1997, 96: 842-848.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Huikuri HV, Makikallio TH, Peng CK, Goldberger AL, Hintze U, Moller M: Fractal correlation properties of R-R interval dynamics and mortality in patients with depressed left ventricular function after an acute myocardial infarction. Circulation 2000, 101: 47-53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Makikallio TH, Seppanen T, Airaksinen KE, Koistinen J, Tulppo MP, Peng CK, Goldberger AL, Huikuri HV: Dynamic analysis of heart rate may predict subsequent ventricular tachycardia after myocardial infarction. Am J Cardiol 1997, 80: 779-783. 10.1016/S0002-9149(97)00516-X

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Makikallio TH, Ristimae T, Airaksinen KE, Peng CK, Goldberger AL, Huikuri HV: Heart rate dynamics in patients with stable angina pectoris and utility of fractal and complexity measures. Am J Cardiol 1998, 81: 27-31. 10.1016/S0002-9149(97)00799-6

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Makikallio TH, Hoiber S, Kober L, Torp-Pedersen C, Peng CK, Goldberger AL, Huikuri HV: Fractal analysis of heart rate dynamics as a predictor of mortality in patients with depressed left ventricular function after acute myocardial infarction. TRACE Investigators. TRAndolapril Cardiac Evaluation. Am J Cardiol 1999, 83: 836-839. 10.1016/S0002-9149(98)01076-5

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Baumert M, Baier V, Haueisen J, Wessel N, Meyerfeldt U, Schirdewan A, Voss A: Forecasting of life threatening arrhythmias using the compression entropy of heart rate. Methods Inf Med 2004, 43: 202-206.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Wessel N, Voss A, Kurths J, Schirdewan A, Hnatkova K, Malik M: Evaluation of renormalised entropy for risk stratification using heart rate variability data. Med Biol Eng Comput 2000, 38: 680-685. 10.1007/BF02344875

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Malatesta D, Simar D, Dauvilliers Y, Candau R, Borrani F, Prefaut C, Caillaud C: Energy cost of walking and gait instability in healthy 65- and 80-yr-olds. J Appl Physiol 2003, 95: 2248-2256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. West BJ, Griffin L: Allometric control of human gait. Fractals-An Interdisciplinary Journal on the Complex Geometry of Nature 1998, 6: 101-108. 10.1142/S0218348X98000122

    Google Scholar 

  56. West BJ, Scafetta N: Nonlinear dynamical model of human gait. Physical Review e 2003., 67:

    Google Scholar 

  57. Kaplan DT, Furman MI, Pincus SM, Ryan SM, Lipsitz LA, Goldberger AL: Aging and the complexity of cardiovascular dynamics. Biophys J 1991, 59: 945-949. 10.1016/S0006-3495(91)82309-8

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Lipsitz LA: Physiological complexity, aging, and the path to frailty. Sci Aging Knowledge Environ 2004, 2004: e16. 10.1126/sageke.2004.16.pe16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Lipsitz LA, Goldberger AL: Loss of 'complexity' and aging. Potential applications of fractals and chaos theory to senescence. JAMA 1992, 267: 1806-1809. 10.1001/jama.267.13.1806

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Ivanov PC, Hausdorff JM, Havlin S, Amaral LA, Arai K, Shulte-Frolinde V, Yoneyama M, Stanley HE: Levels of complexity in scale-invariant neural signals. [] http://arxiv org/abs/cond-mat/0409545http://www.arxiv.org/abs/cond-mat/0409545 2004.http://www.arxiv.org/abs/cond-mat/0409545

    Google Scholar 

  61. Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L, Pieper CF, Leveille SG, Markides KS, Ostir GV, Studenski S, Berkman LF, Wallace RB: Lower extremity function and subsequent disability: consistency across studies, predictive models, and value of gait speed alone compared with the short physical performance battery. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2000, 55: M221-M231.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Aminian K, Najafi B, Bula C, Leyvraz PF, Robert P: Spatio-temporal parameters of gait measured by an ambulatory system using miniature gyroscopes. Journal of Biomechanics 2002, 35: 689-699. 10.1016/S0021-9290(02)00008-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Brach JS, Berthold R, Craik R, VanSwearingen JM, Newman AB: Gait variability in community-dwelling older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2001, 49: 1646-1650. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2001.49274.x

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Owings TM, Grabiner MD: Measuring step kinematic variability on an instrumented treadmill: how many steps are enough? J Biomech 2003, 36: 1215-1218. 10.1016/S0021-9290(03)00108-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Grabiner PC, Biswas ST, Grabiner MD: Age-related changes in spatial and temporal gait variables. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001, 82: 31-35. 10.1053/apmr.2001.18219

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Hausdorff JM, Ladin Z, Wei JY: Footswitch system for measurement of the temporal parameters of gait. J Biomech 1995, 28: 347-351. 10.1016/0021-9290(94)00074-E

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Mayagoitia RE, Nene AV, Veltink PH: Accelerometer and rate gyroscope measurement of kinematics: an inexpensive alternative to optical motion analysis systems. Journal of Biomechanics 2002, 35: 537-542. 10.1016/S0021-9290(01)00231-7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Moe-Nilssen R, Helbostad JL: Interstride trunk acceleration variability but not step width variability can differentiate between fit and frail older adults. Gait & Posture 2005, 21: 164-170. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2004.01.013

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Zijlstra W: Assessment of spatio-temporal parameters during unconstrained walking. European Journal of Applied Physiology 2004, 92: 39-44. 10.1007/s00421-004-1041-5

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Bonato P: Advances in wearable technology and applications in physical medicine and rehabilitation. J Neuroengineering Rehabil 2005, 2: 2. 10.1186/1743-0003-2-2

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Ashkenazy Y, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov PC, Stanley HE: A stochastic model of human gait dynamics. Physica A 2002, 316: 662-670. 10.1016/S0378-4371(02)01453-X

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Hausdorff JM, Ashkenazy Y, Peng CK, Ivanov PC, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL: When human walking becomes random walking: fractal analysis and modeling of gait rhythm fluctuations. Physica A 2001, 302: 138-147. 10.1016/S0378-4371(01)00460-5

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM: Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson's disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci 2005, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Springer S, Giladi N, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM: Deficits in executive function in idiopathic elderly fallers: Association with fall risk. Neurology 2004, 62: A129-A129. 10.1159/000080030

    Google Scholar 

  75. Hausdorff JM, Balash J, Giladi N: Effects of cognitive challenge on gait variability in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2003, 16: 53-58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Hausdorff JM, Yogev G, Springer S, Simon ES, Giladi N: Walking is more like catching than tapping: gait in the elderly as a complex cognitive task. Exp Brain Res 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Danion F, Varraine E, Bonnard M, Pailhous J: Stride variability in human gait: the effect of stride frequency and stride length. Gait Posture 2003, 18: 69-77. 10.1016/S0966-6362(03)00030-4

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Hausdorff JM: Stride variability: beyond length and frequency. Gait Posture 2004, 20: 304. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2003.08.002

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Yamasaki M, Sasaki T, Torii M: Sex difference in the pattern of lower limb movement during treadmill walking. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 1991, 62: 99-103. 10.1007/BF00626763

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Holt KG, Hamill J, Andres RO: Predicting the minimal energy costs of human walking. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1991, 23: 491-498.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Holt KJ, Jeng SF, RR RR, Hamill J: Energetic Cost and Stability During Human Walking at the Preferred Stride Velocity. J Mot Behav 1995, 27: 164-178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Plotnik M, Giladi N, Balash Y, Peretz C, Hausdorff JM: Is freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease related to asymmetric motor function? Ann Neurol 2005, 57: 656-663. 10.1002/ana.20452

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology Circulation 1996, 93: 1043-1065.

  84. Newell KE, Corcos DM: Variability and motor control. Human Kinetics Publishers; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Riley MA, Turvey MT: Variability and determinism in motor behavior. Journal of Motor Behavior 2002, 34: 99-125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Domkin D, Laczko J, Djupsjobacka M, Jaric S, Latash ML: Joint angle variability in 3D bimanual pointing: uncontrolled manifold analysis. Exp Brain Res 2005, 163: 44-57. 10.1007/s00221-004-2137-1

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Latash ML: Progress in motor control, volume one: Bernsteins's traditions in movement studies. 1st edition. Human Kinetics Publishers; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Grobstein P: Variability in brain function and behavior.In The Encyclopedia of Human Behavior, Volume 4 Edited by: Ramachandran VS. Academic Press Inc.; 1994, 447-458. [http://serendip.brynmawr.edu/bb/EncyHumBehav.html]

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by NIH grants AG14100, HD39838, RR13622, and AG08812, from the National Parkinson Foundation and from the US-Israel BiNational Science Foundation. The author thanks Drs. Nir Giladi and Ary L. Goldberger for invaluable discussions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jeffrey M Hausdorff.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hausdorff, J.M. Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 2, 19 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-2-19

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-2-19

Keywords